Saturday, May 24, 2025
LBNN
  • Business
  • Markets
  • Politics
  • Crypto
  • Finance
  • Energy
  • Technology
  • Taxes
  • Creator Economy
  • Wealth Management
  • Documentaries
No Result
View All Result
LBNN

Children’s Views on Inequality: The Power of Explanation

Simon Osuji by Simon Osuji
August 22, 2023
in Artificial Intelligence
0
Children’s Views on Inequality: The Power of Explanation
0
SHARES
2
VIEWS
Share on FacebookShare on Twitter


Summary: Researchers revealed that children’s perceptions of inequality are shaped by how its origins are explained to them.

While adults often attribute social inequalities to structural forces, children interpret these structures differently. However, when children are provided specific explanations about the driving forces behind inequality, their viewpoints align more closely with adult perspectives.

Related posts

Anker Nebula X1 Home Projector Review: Gorgeous Anywhere

Anker Nebula X1 Home Projector Review: Gorgeous Anywhere

May 24, 2025
AI may soon account for half of data center power use if trends persist

AI may soon account for half of data center power use if trends persist

May 24, 2025

This shift in understanding also reduces the bias children hold against lower-status economic groups.

Key Facts:

  1. Children as young as 5 years old can recognize economic inequality and often develop biases favoring high-status groups.
  2. Explanations attributing inequality to structural forces, especially when a high-status group is identified as the originator, can reduce children’s biases.
  3. The research utilized fictional groups to understand children’s attitudes and diminish real-world bias, revealing the influence of causative explanations on perceptions of inequality.

Source: NYU

Children’s views of inequality may be influenced by how its causes are explained to them, finds a new study by a team of psychology researchers.

The work offers insights into the factors that affect how larger social issues are perceived at a young age and points to new ways to reduce bias toward lower-status economic groups.

“When making sense of social inequalities, adults may consider the structural forces at play—for example, people may cite policies related to legacy admissions when thinking about how disparities first arise,” says Rachel Leshin, a New York University doctoral student and the lead author of the study, which appears in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS).

This shows a group of children.
The researchers sought to understand whether and to what extent these explanations would shape children’s responses to the inequality, including their level of bias against the low-status economic group. Credit: Neuroscience News

“But children don’t necessarily see differences in status in this way—and when children are prompted to consider the structural forces, they tend to interpret these structures differently from how adults do.”

“However, our work shows that children can think about these matters in a similar manner as adults do if the structures driving inequality are explained to them in specific ways,” she adds.

“Such approaches, we found, also reduced the extent of bias children felt against a lower-income group relative to a higher-income group.” ”

It’s long been shown that children become aware of inequality from a young age and quickly develop status-related biases as a result. For example, they often view more positively those from high-status groups (e.g., those with more material resources or those who belong to groups that they associate with greater wealth) and, moreover, willingly accept group disparities. 

In the PNAS study, Leshin and Marjorie Rhodes, a professor in NYU’s Department of Psychology, examined how children reason about economic inequality in order to understand how the explanations provided for an inequality shaped children’s responses to it, such as how they feel about a low-status group or whether they want to rectify the inequality. In doing so, the work sought to understand how these explanations could be used to reduce biased views against lower-status groups.

To do so, Leshin and Rhodes recruited  more than 200 children, aged 5-10, to participate in an online study. In the study, children learned about two fictional groups—“Toogits” (a high-status group) and “Flurps” (a low-status group).

The authors note that fictional groups are often used in testing children’s attitudes in order to diminish bias linked to “real-world” social categories. These groups were described as differing in wealth and resources, such as:

See this Flurp? This Flurp lives in this house. And you know what else? Grown-up Flurps have jobs that only pay them a little money. Because Flurps don’t have that much money, this Flurp only got a pair of socks for his/her birthday, and he/she didn’t get to have a birthday party at all.

The children were also shown images that represented where the two groups lived, with the Toogit shown in a nice, polished house and the Flurp shown in a less attractive house.

In order to unpack how the causes provided to explain the inequality shaped children’s responses to it, the researchers gave children one of three explanations for the inequality shown through the two fictional groups: one attributed it to structural causes and cited the high-status group as the structures’ creators (i.e., “… because of rules that [the high-status group] made up a long time ago”); another attributed it to structural causes but did not identify their creator (i.e., “… because of rules that were made up a long time ago”); and one, the control condition, didn’t provide an explanation at all (i.e., “… it’s been like that for a long time”). 

The researchers sought to understand whether and to what extent these explanations would shape children’s responses to the inequality, including their level of bias against the low-status economic group. 

The results showed that only the structural explanation that identified the high-status group as the catalyst responsible for the different circumstances of the two groups produced notable effects.

Children in this condition reported lower levels of bias toward these fictional groups, perceived the status hierarchy as less fair, and opted to give more resources to the low-status group relative to those in the other two conditions. 

By contrast, children who heard a structural explanation that did not cite the high-status group as a cause for these differences (instead citing a third-party—i.e., “the person who got to make the rules”) responded no differently than did those in the control condition who heard no explanation at all. 

“In engaging with children about inequality, whether it’s linked to wealth or educational attainment, it’s important to not only identify a structural cause underlying a disparity, such as legacy admissions, but to also identify the group influential in the implementation of those structures,” explains Leshin.

“We think these findings can be used to better understand how we can meaningfully engage with children about inequality.”

Funding: The research was supported by grants from the National Science Foundation (BCS-2017375) and the National Institutes of Health (1F31HD107965, R01HD087672). 

About this neurodevelopment and psychology research news

Author: James Devitt
Source: NYU
Contact: James Devitt – NYU
Image: The image is credited to Neuroscience News

Original Research: The findings will appear in PNAS



Source link

Previous Post

FDA Approves First Vaccine for Pregnant Individuals to Prevent RSV in Infants

Next Post

Eating Small Daily Handful of Nuts Linked to Lower Risk of Depression

Next Post
Eating Small Daily Handful of Nuts Linked to Lower Risk of Depression

Eating Small Daily Handful of Nuts Linked to Lower Risk of Depression

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

RECOMMENDED NEWS

Ethio Telecom and Visa Strengthen Partnership on Digital Finance

Ethio Telecom and Visa Strengthen Partnership on Digital Finance

1 month ago
Describing the function of tRNA splicing endonuclease TSEN based on its structure

Describing the function of tRNA splicing endonuclease TSEN based on its structure

2 years ago
Artificial Intelligence Makes Compelling Connections Puzzles

Artificial Intelligence Makes Compelling Connections Puzzles

9 months ago
Leaf Twig and Thorn Razor Review: A Sharp Single-Edge

Leaf Twig and Thorn Razor Review: A Sharp Single-Edge

3 months ago

POPULAR NEWS

  • Ghana to build three oil refineries, five petrochemical plants in energy sector overhaul

    Ghana to build three oil refineries, five petrochemical plants in energy sector overhaul

    0 shares
    Share 0 Tweet 0
  • When Will SHIB Reach $1? Here’s What ChatGPT Says

    0 shares
    Share 0 Tweet 0
  • Matthew Slater, son of Jackson State great, happy to see HBCUs back at the forefront

    0 shares
    Share 0 Tweet 0
  • Dolly Varden Focuses on Adding Ounces the Remainder of 2023

    0 shares
    Share 0 Tweet 0
  • US Dollar Might Fall To 96-97 Range in March 2024

    0 shares
    Share 0 Tweet 0
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact

© 2023 LBNN - All rights reserved.

No Result
View All Result
  • Home
  • Business
  • Politics
  • Markets
  • Crypto
  • Economics
    • Manufacturing
    • Real Estate
    • Infrastructure
  • Finance
  • Energy
  • Creator Economy
  • Wealth Management
  • Taxes
  • Telecoms
  • Military & Defense
  • Careers
  • Technology
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Investigative journalism
  • Art & Culture
  • Documentaries
  • Quizzes
    • Enneagram quiz
  • Newsletters
    • LBNN Newsletter
    • Divergent Capitalist

© 2023 LBNN - All rights reserved.